djledda.de main
You can not select more than 25 topics Topics must start with a letter or number, can include dashes ('-') and can be up to 35 characters long.
 
 

748 lines
57 KiB

  1. <html>
  2. <head><title>Intelligence and metabolism</title></head>
  3. <body>
  4. <h1>
  5. Intelligence and metabolism
  6. </h1>
  7. <p>
  8. <hr />
  9. <hr />
  10. </p>
  11. <p>
  12. <strong>Appropriate stimulation is an essential part of the developmental process. Inappropriate stimulation
  13. is a stress that deforms the process of growth. Mediators of stress, such as serotonin, can cause
  14. persistent distortions of physiology and behavior.</strong>
  15. </p>
  16. <p>
  17. <strong>Education can either activate or suppress mental energy. If it is mainly obedience training, it
  18. suppresses energy. If it creates social dislocations, it disturbs mental and emotional energy.</strong>
  19. </p>
  20. <p>
  21. <strong>Stress early in life can impair learning, cause aggressive or compulsive behavior, learned
  22. helplessness, shyness, alcoholism, and other problems.</strong>
  23. </p>
  24. <p>
  25. <strong>Serotonin activates the glucocorticoid system, which can produce brain atrophy. Antiserotonin agents
  26. protect against brain atrophy and many other effects of stress. The brain-protecting neurosteroids,
  27. including pregnenolone and progesterone, which are increased by some kinds of stimulation, are decreased
  28. by isolation stress, and in their absence, serotonin and the glucocorticoids are relatively unopposed.
  29. </strong>
  30. </p>
  31. <p>
  32. <strong>Since excess serotonin can cause thrombosis and vasospasms, and the excess cortisol resulting from
  33. hyperserotonemia can weaken blood vessels and the immune system, a person's longevity is likely to be
  34. shortened if something doesn't intervene to alter the patterns induced by stress early in life.</strong>
  35. </p>
  36. <p>
  37. <hr />
  38. <hr />
  39. </p>
  40. <p>
  41. <strong>Baroness Blatch: "My Lords, the levels of achievement are well above the national average of our own
  42. state schools."
  43. </strong>
  44. </p>
  45. <p>
  46. <strong>"This is a school which attained 75 per cent A to C passes in 1998, and 63.9 per cent in 1999. Those
  47. figures are well above national averages. There is no truancy; and there is the highest possible level
  48. of parental satisfaction with the school. When those parents are paying their money and know what they
  49. are paying for, who are we to take a different view about the philosophy of education in a private
  50. school?"</strong>
  51. </p>
  52. <p>
  53. <strong>Comment during debate in House of Lords, June 30, 1999, on Chief Inspector of Schools Woodhead's
  54. threat to close Summerhill, a democratic school which had been started in 1921.
  55. </strong>
  56. </p>
  57. <p>
  58. <strong>In 1927, the government inspectors had recommended that 'all educationalists' should come to
  59. Summerhill to see its 'invaluable' research, which demonstrated that students' development is better
  60. when they regulate themselves and are not required to attend lessons.</strong>
  61. </p>
  62. <p>
  63. <hr />
  64. <hr />
  65. </p>
  66. <p>
  67. Having written about animal intelligence, and the ways in which it is similar to human intelligence, now I
  68. want those ideas to serve as a context for thinking about human intelligence without many of the usual
  69. preconceptions.
  70. </p>
  71. <p>
  72. Intelligence is an interface between physiology and the environment, so it's necessary to think about each
  73. aspect in relation to the other. Things, both biochemical and social, that enhance intelligence enhance life
  74. itself, and vice versa.
  75. </p>
  76. <p>
  77. Psychologists have tried to give their own definitions to words like idiot, imbecile, moron, and genius, but
  78. they have just been refining the clich"s of the culture, in which "dummy" is one of the first words that
  79. kids in the U.S. learn. Many psychologists have tried to create "culture-free" tests of intelligence, making
  80. it clear that they believe in something like innate animal intelligence, though they usually call it
  81. "genetic" intelligence. Other psychometrists have transcended not only biology but even rationality, and
  82. have catalogued the <strong><em>preferences</em></strong>
  83. of people that they define as intelligent, and designed "I.Q. tests" based on the selection of things that
  84. were preferred by "intelligent people." This behavior is remarkably similar to the "psychometry" of the
  85. general culture, in which "smart" people are those who do things the "right" way.
  86. </p>
  87. <p>
  88. About thirty years ago, someone found that the speed with which the iris contracts in response to a flash of
  89. light corresponds very closely to the I.Q. measured by a psychologist using a standard intelligence test.
  90. The devices used to measure reaction time in drivers' education courses also give a good indication of a
  91. person's intelligence, but so does measuring their heart rate, or taking their temperature. Colleges would
  92. probably be embarrassed to admit students on the basis of their temperature (though they commonly award
  93. scholarships on the basis of the ability to throw a ball). Colleges, to the extent that they are serious
  94. about the business of education, are interested in the student's ability to master the culture.
  95. </p>
  96. <p>
  97. The way a person has learned during childhood can shape that person's manner of grasping the culture. To
  98. simply accelerate the learning of a standard curriculum will increase that person's "I.Q." on a conventional
  99. test, but the important issue is whether it is really intelligent to learn and to value the things taught in
  100. those curricula. Some educators say that their purpose is to socialize and indoctrinate the students into
  101. their discipline, others believe their purpose is to help their students to develop their minds. Both of
  102. these approaches may operate within the idea that "the culture" is something like a museum, and that
  103. students should become curators of the collection, or of some part of it. If we see the culture
  104. metaphorically as a mixture of madhouse, prison, factory, and theater, the idea of "developing the student's
  105. mind" will suggest very different methods and different attitudes toward "the curriculum"
  106. </p>
  107. <p>
  108. Even sophisticated people can fall into stereotyped thinking when they write about issues of intelligence.
  109. For example, no one considers it a sign of genius when a slum kid is fluent in both Spanish and English, but
  110. when some of history's brightest people are discussed, the fact that they learned classical Greek at an
  111. early age is always mentioned. No one mentions whether they were competent in idiomatic Spanish.
  112. </p>
  113. <p>
  114. One of the old cultural stereotypes is that child prodigies always "burn out," as if they were consuming a
  115. fixed amount of mental energy at an accelerated rate. (This idea of burn-out is isomorphic with the other
  116. cultural stereotypes relating aging to the "rate of living," for example that people with slow heart beats
  117. will live longer.) Some of the men who have been considered as the world's brightest have, in fact, gone
  118. through a crisis of depression, and Terman's long-term study of bright people found that "maladjustment" did
  119. increase with I.Q., especially among women. But the facts don't support the concept of "burn-out" at all. I
  120. think the facts reveal instead a deep flaw in our ideas of education and professional knowledge.
  121. </p>
  122. <p>
  123. In a world run by corporation executives, university presidents ("football is central to the university's
  124. mission"), congressmen, bankers, oilmen, and agency bureaucrats, people with the intelligence of an ant (a
  125. warm ant) might seem outlandishly intelligent. This is because the benighted self-interest of the
  126. self-appointed ruling class recognizes that objective reality is always a threat to their interests. If
  127. people, for example, realized that estrogen therapy and serotonin-active drugs and x-rays and nuclear power
  128. and atomic bomb tests were beneficial only to those whose wealth and power derive from them, the whole
  129. system would lose stability. Feigned stupidity becomes real stupidity.
  130. </p>
  131. <p>
  132. But apart from ideologically institutionalized stupidity, there are real variations in the ability to learn,
  133. to remember and to apply knowledge, and to solve problems. These variations are generally metabolic
  134. differences, and so will change according to circumstances that affect metabolism. Everyday social
  135. experiences affect metabolism, stimulating and supporting some kinds of brain activity, suppressing and
  136. punishing others. All of the activities in the child's environment are educational, in one way or another.
  137. </p>
  138. <p>
  139. Some of the famous prodigies of history illustrate the importance of ideology in the development of
  140. intellect. Family ideology, passing on the philosophical orientations of parents and their friends, shapes
  141. the way the children are educated.
  142. </p>
  143. <p>
  144. Some of these family traditions can be traced by considering who the child's godfather was. Jeremy Bentham
  145. was John Stuart Mill's godfather, Mill was Bertrand Russell's; Ralph Waldo Emerson was William James'
  146. godfather, James was W. J. Sidis's. Willy Sidis was educated by his parents to demonstrate their theory of
  147. education, which grew out of the philosophies of Emerson and James. His father, Boris Sidis, was a pioneer
  148. in the study of hypnosis, and he believed that suggestion could mobilize the mind's "reserve energy." Willy
  149. learned several languages and advanced mathematics at an early age. After he graduated from Harvard at the
  150. age of 16, he tried teaching math at Rice Institute, but he was displeased by the attitudes of his students
  151. and of the newspaper and magazine writers who made a profession of mocking him. He attended law school at
  152. Harvard, and would have been imprisoned as a conscientious objector if the war hadn't ended.
  153. </p>
  154. <p>
  155. Antisemitism probably played a role in his sense of isolation when he was at Harvard and Rice. In 1912 Henry
  156. Goddard, a pioneer in intelligence testing (and author of <strong><em>The Kallikak Family: A Study in the
  157. Heredity of Feeble- Mindedness</em>)</strong>, administered intelligence tests to immigrants and
  158. determined that 83 percent of Jews and 87 percent of Russians were "feeble-minded." By the standards of the
  159. time, it was highly inappropriate for the child of extremely poor Jewish immigrants from eastern Europe to
  160. be so bright.
  161. </p>
  162. <p>
  163. Sidis hid from the press, and worked as a bookkeeper and clerk, while he studied and wrote. During his years
  164. of obscurity, he wrote books on philosophy and American history. Eventually, the journalists discovered him
  165. again, and after prolonged lawsuits against the magazines for invasion of privacy and slander, he died of a
  166. stroke at the age of 46.
  167. </p>
  168. <p>
  169. Sidis is probably the culture's favorite example of the child prodigy who burns out, but people (Robert
  170. Persig, Buckminster Fuller) who have read his books have said favorable things about them. The journalists'
  171. emphasis on the fact that Sidis never held a prestigious job nicely illustrates their clich" mentality: "If
  172. you're so smart, why aren't you rich?" But throughout history, intelligent nonconformists have supported
  173. themselves as craft-workers or technicians--Socrates as a stone mason, Spinoza as a lens grinder, Blake as
  174. an engraver, Einstein as a patent examiner, for example.
  175. </p>
  176. <p>
  177. In conventional schools (as in conventional society) 10,000 questions go unanswered, not only because a
  178. teacher with many students has no time to answer them, but also because most teachers wouldn't know most of
  179. the answers.
  180. </p>
  181. <p>
  182. The parents of W. J. Sidis and J. S. Mill were remarkably well educated people who, because they dissented
  183. from society's ideology, chose to spend much of their time educating their children. Whenever a question
  184. about Euclidean geometry or Greek grammar occurred to the child, it could be answered immediately. It was
  185. only natural that progress would be fast, but there were more important differences.
  186. </p>
  187. <p>
  188. When questions are answered, curiosity is rewarded, and the person is enlivened. In school, when following
  189. instructions and conforming to a routine is the main business, many questions must go unanswered, and
  190. curiosity is punished by the dulling emptiness of the routine.
  191. </p>
  192. <p>
  193. Some schools are worse than others. For example, slum children were given I.Q. tests when they started
  194. school, and each subsequent year, and their I.Q.s dropped with each year of school. In a stimulating
  195. environment, the reverse can happen, the I.Q. can rise each year. Since the tests aren't "culture free,"
  196. their scores reflected the material that they were being taught, but they undoubtedly also reflected the
  197. increasing boredom and despair of the children in a bad school, or the increasing liveliness of the children
  198. in the stimulating environment.
  199. </p>
  200. <p>
  201. I have spoken with people in recent years who still held the idea of a fixed genetic mental potential, who
  202. believe that poor children fall behind because they are reaching their "genetic limit." For them, the I.Q.
  203. represents an index of intrinsic quality, and is as important as distinguishing between caviar and frogs'
  204. eggs. The rat research of Marion Diamond and others at the University of California, however, showed that
  205. the structure, weight, and biochemistry of a rat's brain changes, according to the amount of environmental
  206. stimulation and opportunity for exploration. This improvement of brain structure and function is passed on
  207. to the next generation, giving it a head-start. It isn't likely that rats are more disposed than humans to
  208. benefit from mental activity, and in the years since Diamond's research there have been many discoveries
  209. showing that brains of all sorts complexify structurally and functionally in response to stimulation.
  210. </p>
  211. <p>
  212. Rats isolated in little boxes, generation after generation--the normal laboratory rats--were the standard,
  213. but now it's known that isolation is a stress that alters brain chemistry and function.
  214. </p>
  215. <p>
  216. Willy Sidis and John Stuart Mill were being stimulated and allowed to develop in one direction, but they
  217. were being isolated from interaction with their peers. When Mill was twenty he went into a depression, and
  218. later he wrote that it was because he discovered that he was unable to <strong><em>feel.</em></strong> He
  219. had developed only part of his personality.
  220. </p>
  221. <p>
  222. Bertrand Russell (1872-1970), orphaned at the age of four, went to live with his grandmother, who chose not
  223. to send him to school, but provided tutors. He didn't experience a sense of academic pressure, and was able
  224. to read whatever he wanted in his late grandfather's library. He didn't realize that he was unusually bright
  225. until he went to Cambridge. The unusual freedom of his childhood must have contributed to his willingness to
  226. hold unpopular opinions. In 1916 he was fined, and in 1918 imprisoned for 6 months, for opposing the war.
  227. </p>
  228. <p>
  229. In 1927, Russell and his wife, Dora Black, started a school. He later wrote that, although the average
  230. student at the school was very bright, an exceptionally bright student was likely to be ostracized by the
  231. less bright students. He commented on the harm done to the brightest students by their social isolation,
  232. probably thinking about his own education in relative isolation. A psychologist (Leta Hollingworth, 1942)
  233. has made similar observations about the isolation that can be produced by a large difference of I.Q. She did
  234. a series of studies of very bright children, beginning in 1916, including working with some of them in a
  235. program she designed in a New York public school. Her empathy allowed her to discover things that weren't
  236. apparent to her contemporaries.
  237. </p>
  238. <p>
  239. During this time Lewis Terman was studying bright children, and wanted to disprove some of the popular
  240. stereotypes about intelligent people, and to support his ideology of white racial superiority. In 1922 he
  241. got a large grant, and sorted out about 1500 of the brightest children from a group of 250,000 in
  242. California. He and his associates then monitored them for the rest of their lives (described in <strong><em
  243. >Genetic Studies of Genius</em></strong>). His work contradicted the stereotype of bright people as
  244. being sickly or frail, but, contrary to his expectation, there was an association between maladjustment and
  245. higher I.Q.; the incidence of neurotic fatigue, anxiety, and depression increased along with the I.Q. The
  246. least bright of his group were more successful in many ways than the most bright. He didn't really confront
  247. the implications of this, though it seriously challenged his belief in a simple genetic racial superiority
  248. of physique, intellect, and character.
  249. </p>
  250. <p>
  251. I.Q. testing originated in a historical setting in which its purpose was often to establish a claim of
  252. racial superiority, or to justify sterilization or "euthanasia," or to exclude immigrants. More recently,
  253. the tests have been used to assign students to certain career paths. Because of their use by people in power
  254. to control others, the I.Q. tests have helped to create misunderstanding of the nature of intelligence. A
  255. person's "I.Q." now has very strong associations with the ideology of schooling as a road to financial
  256. success, rather than to enrichment of a shared mental life.
  257. </p>
  258. <p>
  259. If a bad school resembles, on the intellectual level, a confining rat box, the educational isolation of
  260. Mill, Russell, and Sidis was emotionally limiting, almost like solitary confinement. Once when Willy Sidis
  261. was arrested for marching in a May Day parade, his father was able to keep him from going to prison, but
  262. Willy apparently would have preferred the real prison to life with his parents.
  263. </p>
  264. <p>
  265. None of these three famous intellects was known for youthful playfulness, though playfulness is a quality
  266. that's closely associated with intelligence in mammals and birds. (Russell, however, in middle age developed
  267. many new interests, such as writing short stories, and had many new loves even in old age.) Stress early in
  268. life, such as isolation, reduces the playfulness of experimental animals. Playfulness is contagious, but so
  269. is the inability to play.
  270. </p>
  271. <p>
  272. In schools like Summerhill, which was founded in 1921 by A. S. Neill, students aren't required to attend
  273. classes when they would rather do something else, but at graduation they usually do better on their
  274. standardized national examinations than students who have dutifully attended classes for years. For
  275. students, as for rats, freedom and variety are good for the brain, and tedious conformity is harmful. When a
  276. school is very good, it can spread a contagion of playfulness along with an interest in learning.
  277. </p>
  278. <p>
  279. An environment that fosters optimal intelligence will necessarily promote the development of emotional
  280. health, and will almost certainly foster good physical health and longevity, because no part of the
  281. physiological system can thrive at the expense of another part. And within the boundaries of life-enriching
  282. environments, there are infinite possibilities for variety.
  283. </p>
  284. <p>
  285. There is a common belief in the rigidity of the adult nervous system, in analogy with feral cats or dogs,
  286. that supposedly can't be tamed if they have grown up without knowing humans. But people who have had the
  287. inclination to understand wild animals have found that, even when the animals have been captured as adults,
  288. they can become as sociable as if they had grown up in domestication. The "horse whisperer" demonstrated
  289. this sort of empathetic approach to animals. Sometimes, these people have a similar ability to communicate
  290. with people who are retarded, or autistic, or demented, but the professionalization of society has made it
  291. increasingly unlikely that people with the need for intuitive help will encounter someone who is able to
  292. give it. The closest psychology has come to professionally recognizing the importance of empathy was in Carl
  293. Rogers' work, e.g., <em>
  294. Client-Centered Therapy.</em>
  295. </p>
  296. <p>
  297. Rogers showed that a sense of solidarity must exist between therapist and client for the therapy to be
  298. helpful. A similar solidarity has to exist between teacher and student, for education to be successful. If
  299. ordinary family and social contacts could occur within such an atmosphere of mutual respect, psychopathology
  300. (including learning difficulties) would be much less common.
  301. </p>
  302. <p>
  303. Although three individuals don't prove an argument, I think the lives and situations of Sidis, Mill, and
  304. Russell are usefully symbolic. Sidis, who grew up under intense pressure and social isolation and in extreme
  305. poverty, died at the age of 46. Mill, who was educated mainly by his father, in secure financial
  306. circumstances, experienced social isolation and moderate pressure, and lived about 20 years longer than
  307. Sidis did. Russell, who grew up in the highest circles of the ruling class, experienced no pressure, and
  308. only the mild kind of social isolation that wasn't exceptional for his class. He lived to be 97.
  309. </p>
  310. <p>
  311. The psychopathology of social isolation has been studied in a variety of animals, and many features are
  312. similar across species, including humans. Aggression, helplessness, and reduced ability to learn are
  313. typically produced in animals by social isolation, and it's clear that certain kinds of family environment
  314. produce the same conditions in children. Schools seldom help, and often hinder, recovery from such early
  315. experiences.
  316. </p>
  317. <p>
  318. "Vital exhaustion," decreased slow wave sleep, and anger, which are associated with the "type A personality"
  319. and with circulatory and heart disease, appear to have their origin in childhood experiences. Low income and
  320. financial insecurity are strongly associated with anger, sleep disturbances, and circulatory disease. In
  321. animals stressed by social isolation, similar features emerge, under the influence of decreased
  322. neurosteroids, and increased serotonin and activity of the glucocorticoid system.
  323. </p>
  324. <p>
  325. The "smart drug" culture has generally been thinking pharmaceutically rather than biologically. Behind that
  326. pharmaceutical orientation there is sometimes the idea that the individual just isn't trying hard enough, or
  327. doesn't have quite the right genes to excel mentally.
  328. </p>
  329. <p>
  330. Many stimulants--amphetamine and estrogen, for example--can increase alertness temporarily, but at the
  331. expense of long range damage. The first principle of stimulation should be to avoid a harmful activation of
  332. the catabolic stress hormones. Light, play, environmental variety and exploratory conversations stimulate
  333. the whole organism in an integral way, stimulating repair processes and developmental processes.
  334. </p>
  335. <p>
  336. Any chemical support for intelligence should take into account the mind-damaging stresses that our culture
  337. can impose, and provide defense against those. In darkness and isolation, for example, the stress hormones
  338. increase, and the brain-protective steroids decrease. The memory improvement that results from taking
  339. pregnenolone or thyroid (which is needed for synthesizing pregnenolone from cholesterol) is the result of
  340. turning off the dulling and brain-dissolving stress hormones, allowing normal responsiveness to be restored.
  341. </p>
  342. <p>
  343. If we know that rats nurtured in freedom, in an interesting environment, grow more intelligent, then it
  344. would seem obvious that we should experiment with similar approaches for children--if we are really
  345. interested in fostering intelligence. And since violence and mental dullness are created by the same social
  346. stresses, even the desire to reduce school violence might force the society to make some improvements that
  347. will, as a side effect, foster intelligence.
  348. </p>
  349. <p><h3>REFERENCES</h3></p>
  350. <p>
  351. B. Russell: "If you wish to know what men will do, you must know not only or principally their material
  352. circumstances, but rather the whole system of their desires with their relative strengths."
  353. </p>
  354. <p>
  355. John Holt, from an interview in <em>Mother Earth News,</em> July/August, 1980<strong>:</strong>
  356. "I suggested that we simply provide young people with schools where there are a lot of interesting things to
  357. look at and work with . . . but that we let the chidlren learn in their own wqys. If they have questions,
  358. answer the questions. If they want to know where to look for something, show them where to look."
  359. </p>
  360. <p>
  361. John Holt, from the introduction to his book, <em>Teach Your Own,</em> (New York: Dell, 1981)<strong
  362. >:</strong> "The children in the classroom, despite their rich backgrounds and high I.Q.'s, were with few
  363. exceptions frightened, timid, evasive, and self-protecting. The infants at home were bold adventurers."
  364. </p>
  365. <p>
  366. "It soon became clear to me that children are by nature and from birth very curious about the world around
  367. them, and very energetic, resourceful, and competent in exploring it, finding out about it, and mastering.
  368. In short, much more eager to learn, and much better at learning, than most adults. Babies are not blobs, but
  369. true scientists. Why not then make schools into places in which children would be allowed, encouraged, and
  370. (if and when they asked) helped to explore and make sense of the world around them (in time and space) in
  371. ways that most interested them?"
  372. </p>
  373. <p>
  374. Psychosom Med 1984 Nov-Dec;46(6):546-8. <strong>Rapid communication: whole blood serotonin and the type A
  375. behavior pattern.</strong> Madsen D, McGuire MT.<strong>
  376. In 72 young males, whole blood serotonin is shown to have a pronounced relationship with the Type A
  377. behavior pattern.</strong> The relationship is explored with multivariate statistical techniques.
  378. </p>
  379. <p>
  380. J Neurochem. 2000 Aug;75(2):732-40. Serra M, Pisu MG, Littera M, Papi G, Sanna E, Tuveri F, Usala L, Purdy
  381. RH, Biggio G.<strong>
  382. Social isolation-induced decreases in both the abundance of neuroactive steroids and GABA(A) receptor
  383. function in rat brain.</strong>
  384. </p>
  385. <p>
  386. Ann Med 2000 Apr;32(3):210-21. <strong>Role of serotonin in memory impairment.</strong> Buhot MC, Martin S,
  387. Segu L.
  388. </p>
  389. <p>
  390. Ivan Illich and Etienne Verne, <strong><em>Imprisoned in the global classroom.</em></strong>
  391. London, Writers and Readers Publishing Cooperative, 1976.
  392. </p>
  393. <p>
  394. Ivan Illich, <strong><em>Deschooling society.</em></strong> Harmondsworth: Penguin, 1976 (1971).
  395. </p>
  396. <p>
  397. ----Tools for Conviviality<em> </em>(1973).
  398. </p>
  399. <p><strong><em>----Toward a history of needs.</em></strong> New York, Pantheon Books, c1978.</p>
  400. <p>
  401. ----Limits to medicine. medical nemesis : the expropriation of health. Harmondsworth New York, Penguin,
  402. 1977.
  403. </p>
  404. <p>
  405. <strong><em>----Celebration of awareness: a call for institutional revolution.</em></strong> Harmondsworth,
  406. Penguin Education, 1976. Pelican books Originally published: Garden City [N.Y.]: Doubleday, 1970; London:
  407. Calder and Boyars, 1971.
  408. </p>
  409. <p><strong><em>----Disabling professions.</em></strong> London, Boyars, 1977, Ideas in progress series.</p>
  410. <p>
  411. Eur J Pharmacol 1992 Feb 25;212(1):73-8. <strong>5-HT3 receptor antagonists reverse helpless behaviour in
  412. rats.</strong> Martin P, Gozlan H, Puech AJ Departement de Pharmacologie, Faculte de Medecine
  413. Pitie-Salpetriere, Paris, France. The effects of the 5-HT3 receptor antagonists, zacopride, ondansetron and
  414. ICS 205-930, were investigated in an animal model of depression, the learned helplessness test. Rats
  415. previously subjected to a session of 60 inescapable foot-shocks exhibited a deficit of escape performance in
  416. three subsequent shuttle-box sessions. The 5-HT3 receptor antagonists administered i.p. twice daily on a
  417. chronic schedule (zacopride 0.03-2 mg/kg per day; ondansetron and ICS 205-930: 0.125-2 mg/kg per day)
  418. reduced the number of escape failures at low to moderate daily doses. This effect was not observed with the
  419. highest dose(s) of zacopride, ondansetron and ICS 205-930 tested.. These results indicate that 5-HT3
  420. antagonists may have effects like those of conventional antidepressants in rats.
  421. </p>
  422. <p>
  423. Neuropharmacology 1992 Apr;31(4):323-30. <strong>Presynaptic serotonin mechanisms in rats subjected to
  424. inescapable shock.</strong> Edwards E, Kornrich W, Houtten PV, Henn FA. "After exposure to
  425. uncontrollable shock training, two distinct groups of rats can be defined in terms of their performance in
  426. learning to escape from a controllable stress. Learned helpless rats do not learn to terminate the
  427. controllable stress, whereas non-learned helpless rats learn this response as readily as naive control rats
  428. do." "These results implicate presynaptic serotonin mechanisms in the behavioral deficit caused by
  429. uncontrollable shock. In addition, a limbic-hypothalamic pathway may serve as a control center for the
  430. behavioral response to stress."
  431. </p>
  432. <p>
  433. Neurochem Int 1992 Jul;21(1):29-35.<strong>
  434. In vitro neurotransmitter release in an animal model of depression</strong>. Edwards E, Kornrich W, van
  435. Houtten P, Henn FA. "Sprague-Dawley rats exposed to uncontrollable shock can be separated by a subsequent
  436. shock escape test into two groups: a "helpless" (LH) group which demonstrates a deficit in escape behavior,
  437. and a "nonlearned helpless" (NLH) group which shows no escape deficit and acquires the escape response as
  438. readily as naive control rats (NC) do." "The major finding concerned a significant increase in endogenous
  439. and K(+)-stimulated serotonin (5-HT) release in the hippocampal slices of LH rats. There were no apparent
  440. differences in acetylcholine, dopamine and noradrenaline release in the hippocampus of LH rats as compared
  441. to NLH and NC rats. These results add further support to previous studies in our laboratory which implicate
  442. presynaptic 5-HT mechanisms in the behavioral deficit caused by uncontrollable shock."
  443. </p>
  444. <p>
  445. Psychiatry Res 1994 Jun;52(3):285-93. <strong>In vivo serotonin release and learned helplessness.</strong>
  446. Petty F, Kramer G, Wilson L, Jordan S Mental Health Clinic, Dallas Veterans Affairs Medical Center, TX.
  447. Learned helplessness, a behavioral depression caused by exposure to inescapable stress, is considered to be
  448. an animal model of human depressive disorder. Like human depression, learned helplessness has been
  449. associated with a defect in serotonergic function, but the nature of this relationship is not entirely
  450. clear. We have used in vivo microdialysis brain perfusion to measure serotonin (5-hydroxytryptamine, 5HT) in
  451. extracellular space of medial frontal cortex in conscious, freely moving rats. Basal 5HT levels in rats
  452. perfused before exposure to tail-shock stress did not themselves correlate with subsequent learned
  453. helplessness behavior. However, 5HT release after stress showed a significant increase with helpless
  454. behavior. <strong>These data support the hypothesis that a cortical serotonergic excess is causally related
  455. to the development of learned helplessness.</strong>
  456. </p>
  457. <p>
  458. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1994 Jul;48(3):671-6. <strong>Does learned helplessness induction by haloperidol
  459. involve serotonin mediation?</strong> Petty F, Kramer G, Moeller M Veterans Affairs Medical Center,
  460. Dallas 75216. Learned helplessness (LH) is a behavioral depression following inescapable stress. Helpless
  461. behavior was induced in naive rats by the dopamine D2 receptor blocker haloperidol (HDL) in a dose-dependent
  462. manner, with the greatest effects seen at 20 mg/kg (IP). Rats were tested 24 h after injection. Haloperidol
  463. (IP) increased release of serotonin (5-HT) in medial prefrontal cortex (MPC) as measured by in vivo
  464. microdialysis. Perfusion of HDL through the probe in MPC caused increased cortical 5-HT release, as did
  465. perfusion of both dopamine and the dopamine agonist apomorphine. Our previous work found that increased 5-HT
  466. release in MPC correlates with the development of LH. The present work suggests that increased DA release in
  467. MPC, known to occur with both inescapable stress and with HDL, may play a necessary but not sufficient role
  468. in the development of LH. Also, this suggests that increased DA activity in MPC leads to increased 5-HT
  469. release in MPC and to subsequent behavioral depression.
  470. </p>
  471. <p>
  472. Arzneimittelforschung 1975 Nov; 25(11):1737-44<strong>. [Central action of WA-335-BS, a substance with
  473. peripheral antiserotonin and antihistaminic activity].</strong> Kahling J, Ziegler H, Ballhause H. "In
  474. rats and mice the serotonin and histamine antagonistic drug <strong>. . .</strong> (WA 335-BS) caused
  475. stronger central sedative effects than did cyproheptadine. WA 335-BS also displayed stronger activity
  476. against reserpine- and central tremorine-induced effects than did cyproheptadine and it slightly enhanced
  477. d-amphetamine-induced<strong> </strong>
  478. effects:<strong>
  479. therefore it may have antidepressant properties. WA 335-BS proved to be</strong>
  480. <strong>very effective against isolation-induced aggression in male mice.</strong> The comparatively small
  481. anxiolytic effects may have been caused in part by the central antiserotonin properties." "The results of
  482. our animal studies suggest WA 335-BS to be an antidepressant with sedative properties."
  483. </p>
  484. <p>
  485. Neuroscience 2000;100(4):749-68<strong>. Behavioral, neurochemical and endocrinological characterization of
  486. the early social isolation syndrome.</strong> Heidbreder CA, Weiss IC, Domeney AM, Pryce C, Homberg J,
  487. Hedou G, Feldon J, Moran MC, Nelson P. "Rearing rats in isolation has been shown to be a relevant paradigm
  488. for studying early life stress and<strong>
  489. understanding the genesis of depression and related affective disorders.</strong> Recent studies from
  490. our laboratory point to the relevance of studying the social isolation syndrome as a function of home caging
  491. conditions."
  492. </p>
  493. <p>
  494. Stroke 1991 Nov;22(11):1448-51. <strong>Platelet secretory products may contribute to neuronal
  495. injury.</strong> Joseph R, Tsering C, Grunfeld S, Welch KM. BACKGROUND: We do not fully understand the
  496. mechanisms for neuronal damage following cerebral arterial occlusion by a thrombus that consists mainly of
  497. platelets. The view that certain endogenous substances, such as glutamate, may also contribute to neuronal
  498. injury is now reasonably well established. Blood platelets are known to contain and secrete a number of
  499. substances that have been associated with neuronal dysfunction. Therefore, we hypothesize that a high
  500. concentration (approximately several thousand-fold higher than in plasma, in our estimation) of locally
  501. released platelet secretory products derived from the causative thrombus may contribute to neuronal injury
  502. and promote reactive gliosis. SUMMARY OF COMMENT: We have recently been able to report some direct support
  503. for this concept. When organotypic spinal cord cultures were exposed to platelet and platelet products, a
  504. significant reduction in the number and the size of the surviving neurons occurred in comparison with those
  505. in controls. We further observed that serotonin, a major platelet product, has neurotoxic properties. There
  506. may be other platelet components with similar effect.<strong>
  507. CONCLUSIONS: The hypothesis of platelet-mediated neurotoxicity gains some support from these recent in
  508. vitro findings. The concept could provide a new area of research in stroke, both at the clinical and
  509. basic levels.</strong>
  510. </p>
  511. <p>
  512. Am J Psychiatry 1981 Aug;138(8):1082-5.<strong>
  513. Tryptophan metabolism in children with attentional deficit disorder.</strong> Irwin M, Belendiuk K,
  514. McCloskey K, Freedman DX The authors present the first report, to their knowledge, of hyperserotonemia in
  515. children with attentional deficit disorder who had normal intelligence. Hyperserotonemic children had
  516. significantly lower levels of plasma total and protein-bound tryptophan and a higher percentage of free
  517. tryptophan than those with normal serotonin levels. Plasma kynurenine did not differ, suggesting that the
  518. hyperserotonemia is not due to a blockade of the kynurenine pathway but may reflect on increase in tissue
  519. tryptophan uptake and use.
  520. </p>
  521. <p>
  522. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 1990 Summer;2(3):268-74.<strong>
  523. Autistic children and their first-degree relatives: relationships between serotonin and norepinephrine
  524. levels and intelligence.</strong> Cook EH, Leventhal BL, Heller W, Metz J, Wainwright M, Freedman DX
  525. "Whole-blood serotonin (5-HT) and plasma norepinephrine (NE) were studied in 16 autistic children, 21
  526. siblings of autistic children, and 53 parents of autistic children. <strong>Both plasma NE and whole-blood
  527. 5-HT were negatively correlated with vocabulary performance."
  528. </strong>
  529. "Eighteen subjects were hyperserotonemic (whole-blood 5-HT greater than 270 ng/ml). For these subjects,
  530. plasma NE was significantly higher than for subjects without hyperserotonemia."
  531. </p>
  532. <p>
  533. Biol Psychiatry 1998 Dec 15;44(12):1321-8. <strong>Cerebrospinal fluid monoamines in Prader-Willi
  534. syndrome.</strong> Akefeldt A, Ekman R, Gillberg C, Mansson JE "The behavioral phenotype of Prader-Willi
  535. syndrome (PWS) suggests hypothalamic dysfunction and altered neurotransmitter regulation. The purpose of
  536. this study was to examine whether there was any difference in the concentrations of monoamine metabolites in
  537. the cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) in PWS and non-PWS comparison cases." "The concentrations of<strong>
  538. dopamine and particularly serotonin metabolites were increased in the PWS group. The differences were
  539. most prominent for 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid. The increased concentrations were found in all PWS cases
  540. independently of age, body mass index, and level of mental retardation." "The findings implicate
  541. dysfunction of the serotonergic system and possibly also of the dopamine system
  542. </strong>
  543. in PWS individuals . . . ."
  544. </p>
  545. <p>
  546. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1976 Jul;5(1):55-61. <strong>The role of serotonergic pathways in isolation-induced
  547. aggression in mice.</strong> Malick JB, Barnett A Male mice that became aggressive following four weeks
  548. of social isolation were treated with seven known serotonin receptor antagonists. All of the<strong>
  549. antiserotonergic drugs selectively antagonized the fighting behavior of the isolated mice; the
  550. antiaggressive activity was selective since, at antifighting doses, none of the drugs either
  551. significantly altered spontaneous motor activity</strong>
  552. or impaired inclined-screen performance. <strong>Antagonism of 5-HTP-induced head-twitch was used as an in
  553. vivo measure of antiserotonergic activity and a statistically significant correlation existed between
  554. potency as an antiserotonergic and potency as an antiaggressive.</strong> PCPA, a serotonin depletor,
  555. also significantly <strong>antagonized isolation-induced aggression</strong> for at least 24 hr postdrug
  556. administration. The interrelationship between cholinergic and serotonergic mechanisms in the mediation of
  557. isolation aggression was investigated. The involvement of serotonergic systems in isolation-induced
  558. aggression is discussed.
  559. </p>
  560. <p>
  561. Probl Endokrinol (Mosk) 1979 May-Jun;25(3):49-52<strong>
  562. [Role of serotonin receptors of the medial-basal hypothalamus in the mechanisms of negative feedback of
  563. the hypophyseal-testicular complex].</strong> Naumenko EV, Shishkina GT. "Administration of serotonin
  564. into the lateral ventricle of the brain of male rats, against the background of complete isolation of the
  565. medial-basal hypothalamus was accompanied by the block of the compensatory elevation of the blood
  566. testosterone level following unilateral castration."
  567. </p>
  568. <p>
  569. Encephale 1994 Sep-Oct;20(5):521-5. <strong>[Can a serotonin uptake agonist be an authentic antidepressant?
  570. Results of a multicenter, multinational therapeutic trial].</strong> Kamoun A, Delalleau B, Ozun M The
  571. classical biochemical hypothesis of depression posits a functional deficit in central neurotransmitter
  572. systems particularly serotonin (5-HT) and noradrenaline. The major role suggested for 5-HT in this theory
  573. led to the development of a large number of compounds which selectively inhibit 5-HT uptake. Numerous
  574. clinical trials have demonstrated the antidepressant efficacy of such types of serotoninergic agents,
  575. supporting 5-HT deficit as the main origin of depression. <strong>Therefore, everything seemed clear:
  576. depression was caused by 5-HT deficit. Tianeptine is clearly active in classical animal models
  577. predictive of antidepressant activity, and is also active in behavioral screening tests: it antagonizes
  578. isolation induced aggression in mice and behavioral despair in rats.</strong> Biochemical studies have
  579. revealed that in contrast to classical tricyclic antidepressant,<strong>
  580. tianeptine stimulates 5-HT uptake
  581. </strong>
  582. in vivo in the rat brain. This somewhat surprising property was observed in the cortex and the hippocampus
  583. following both acute and chronic administrations. This increase in 5-HT uptake has also been confirmed in
  584. rat platelets after acute and<strong>
  585. chronic administrations. Moreover, in humans, a study in depressed patients demonstrated that tianeptine
  586. significantly increased platelet 5-HT uptake after a single administration as well as after 10 and 28
  587. days of treatment. The antidepressant activity of tianeptine has been evaluated in controlled studies
  588. versus reference antidepressants. Another study aiming to compare the antidepressant efficacy of
  589. tianeptine versus placebo and versus imiporamine is</strong> presented. 186 depressed patients were
  590. included in this trial. They presented with either Major Depression, single episode (24.6%) or Major
  591. Depression recurrent (66.8%) or Bipolar Disorder (depressed) (8.6%).
  592. </p>
  593. <p>
  594. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1998 Oct;139(3):255-60.<strong>
  595. Ca2+ dependency of serotonin and dopamine release from CNS slices of chronically isolated rats.</strong>
  596. Jaffe EH. "We have used chronic isolated housing as an animal model of depression." "The following questions
  597. were addressed: first, if there is a change in the depolarization dependent release of DA and 5-HT from
  598. these CNS structures, and second, if the release is through the classical exocytotic mechanism. <strong>A
  599. significant increase in KCl stimulated release of 5-HT was observed in chronically isolated animals when
  600. compared to controls.
  601. </strong>
  602. 5-HT release was completely abolished from controls or isolated animals, when slices were incubated with
  603. Krebs containing zero Ca2+/10 mM Mg2+, the inorganic Ca2+ channel blockers, Cd2+ or Ni2+ and the calmodulin
  604. inhibitor, trifluoperazine." <strong>"The basal release of DA and 5-HT was similar in control and isolated
  605. animals and was not affected by the Ca2+ channel antagonists. The results suggest that extracellular
  606. Ca2+-dependent release of 5-HT and, to a lesser degree, of DA, is increased in this chronic animal model
  607. of depression in</strong> several CNS structures."
  608. </p>
  609. <p>
  610. Gen Pharmacol 1994 Oct;25(6):1257-1262.<strong>
  611. Serotonin-induced decrease in brain ATP, stimulation of brain anaerobic glycolysis and elevation of
  612. plasma hemoglobin; the protective action of calmodulin antagonists.</strong> Koren-Schwartzer N,
  613. Chen-Zion M, Ben-Porat H, Beitner R Department of Life Sciences, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat Gan, Israel.
  614. <strong>1. Injection of serotonin (5-hydroxytryptamine) to rats, induced a dramatic fall in brain ATP level,
  615. accompanied by an increase in P(i). Concomitant to these changes, the activity of cytosolic
  616. phosphofructokinase, the rate-limiting enzyme of glycolysis, was significantly enhanced. Stimulation of
  617. anaerobic glycolysis was also reflected by a marked increase in lactate content in brain. 2. Brain
  618. glucose</strong> 1,6-bisphosphate level was decreased, whereas fructose 2,6-bisphosphate was unaffected
  619. by serotonin. 3. All these serotonin-induced changes in brain, which are characteristic for cerebral
  620. ischemia, were prevented by treatment with the calmodulin (CaM) antagonists, trifluoperazine or
  621. thioridazine. 4<strong>.. Injection of serotonin also induced a marked elevation of plasma hemoglobin,
  622. reflecting lysed erythrocytes,</strong> which was also prevented by treatment with the CaM antagonists.
  623. 5.<strong>
  624. The present results suggest that CaM antagonists may be effective drugs in treatment of many
  625. pathological conditions and diseases in which plasma serotonin levels are known to increase.</strong>
  626. </p>
  627. <p>
  628. Gen Pharmacol 1994 Oct;25(6):1257-1262.<strong>
  629. Serotonin-induced decrease in brain ATP, stimulation of brain anaerobic glycolysis and elevation of
  630. plasma hemoglobin; the protective action of calmodulin antagonists.</strong> Koren-Schwartzer N,
  631. Chen-Zion M, Ben-Porat H, Beitner R Department of Life Sciences, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat Gan, Israel.
  632. <strong>1. Injection of serotonin (5-hydroxytryptamine) to rats, induced a dramatic fall in brain ATP level,
  633. accompanied by an increase in P(i). Concomitant to these changes, the activity of cytosolic
  634. phosphofructokinase, the rate-limiting enzyme of glycolysis, was significantly enhanced. Stimulation of
  635. anaerobic glycolysis was also reflected by a marked increase in lactate content in brain. 2. Brain
  636. glucose</strong> 1,6-bisphosphate level was decreased, whereas fructose 2,6-bisphosphate was unaffected
  637. by serotonin. 3. All these serotonin-induced changes in brain, which are characteristic for cerebral
  638. ischemia, were prevented by treatment with the calmodulin (CaM) antagonists, trifluoperazine or
  639. thioridazine. 4. Injection of serotonin also induced a marked elevation of plasma hemoglobin, reflecting
  640. lysed erythrocytes, which was also prevented by treatment with the CaM antagonists. 5. The present results
  641. suggest that CaM antagonists may be effective drugs in treatment of many pathological conditions and
  642. diseases in which plasma serotonin levels are known to increase.
  643. </p>
  644. <p>
  645. J Neural Transm 1998;105(8-9):975-86. <strong>Role of tryptophan in the elevated serotonin-turnover in
  646. hepatic encephalopathy.</strong> Herneth AM, Steindl P, Ferenci P, Roth E, Hortnagl H. "The increase of
  647. the brain levels of 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid (5-HIAA) in hepatic encephalopathy (HE) suggests an increased
  648. turnover of serotonin (5-HT)." "These results provide further evidence for the role of tryptophan in the
  649. elevation of brain 5-HT metabolism and for a potential role of BCAA in the treatment of HE."
  650. </p>
  651. <p>
  652. Tugai VA; Kurs'kii MD; Fedoriv OM. <strong>[Effect of serotonin on Ca2+ transport in mitochondria conjugated
  653. with the respiratory chain].</strong> Ukrainskii Biokhimicheskii Zhurnal, 1973 Jul-Aug, 45(4):408-12.
  654. </p>
  655. <p>
  656. Kurskii MD; Tugai VA; Fedoriv AN.<strong>
  657. [Effect of serotonin and calcium on separate components of respiratory chain of mitochondria in some
  658. rabbit tissues].</strong>
  659. Ukrainskii Biokhimicheskii Zhurnal, 1970, 42(5):584-8.
  660. </p>
  661. <p>
  662. Watanabe Y; Shibata S; Kobayashi B. <strong>Serotonin-induced swelling of rat liver mitochondria.</strong>
  663. Endocrinologia Japonica, 1969 Feb, 16(1):133-47.
  664. </p>
  665. <p>
  666. Mahler DJ; Humoller FL. <strong>The influence of serotonin on oxidative metabolism of brain
  667. mitochondria.</strong> Proceedings of the Society for Experimental Biology and Medicine, 1968 Apr,
  668. 127(4):1074-9.
  669. </p>
  670. <p>
  671. Eur J Pharmacol 1994 Aug 11;261(1-2):25-32. <strong>The effect of alpha 2-adrenoceptor antagonists in
  672. isolated globally ischemic rat hearts.</strong> Sargent CA, Dzwonczyk S, Grover G.J. "The alpha
  673. 2-adrenoceptor antagonist, yohimbine, has been reported to protect hypoxic myocardium. Yohimbine has several
  674. other activities, including 5-HT receptor antagonism, at the concentrations at which protection was found."
  675. "Pretreatment with yohimbine (1-10 microM) caused a concentration-dependent increase in reperfusion left
  676. ventricular developed pressure and a reduction in end diastolic pressure and lactate dehydrogenase release.
  677. The structurally similar compound rauwolscine (10 microM) also protected the ischemic myocardium. In
  678. contrast, idozoxan (0.3-10 microM) or tolazoline (10 microM) had no protective effects. The<strong>
  679. cardioprotective effects of yohimbine were partially reversed by 30 microM 5-HT. These results indicate
  680. that the mechanism for the cardioprotective activity of yohimbine may involve 5-HT receptor antagonistic
  681. activity."
  682. </strong>
  683. </p>
  684. <p>
  685. Zubovskaia AM. <strong>[Effect of serotonin on some pathways of oxidative metabolism in the mitochondria of
  686. rabbit heart muscle].</strong> Voprosy Meditsinskoi Khimii, 1968 Mar-Apr, 14(2):152-7.
  687. </p>
  688. <p>
  689. Warashina Y. <strong>
  690. [On the effect of serotonin on phosphorylation of rat liver mitochondria</strong>]. Hoppe-Seylers
  691. Zeitschrift fur Physiologische Chemie, 1967 Feb, 348(2):139-48.
  692. </p>
  693. <p>
  694. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 1997 Oct;7 Suppl 3:S323-S328. <strong>Prevention of stress-induced morphological
  695. and cognitive consequences</strong>.. McEwen BS, Conrad CD, Kuroda Y, Frankfurt M, Magarinos AM,
  696. McKittrick C. Atrophy and dysfunction of the human hippocampus is a feature of aging in some individuals,
  697. and this dysfunction predicts later dementia. There is reason to believe that adrenal glucocorticoids may
  698. contribute to these changes, since the elevations of glucocorticoids in Cushing's syndrome and during normal
  699. aging are associated with atrophy of the entire hippocampal formation in humans and are linked to deficits
  700. in short-term verbal memory. We have developed a model of stress-induced atrophy of the hippocampus of rats
  701. at the cellular level, and we have been investigating underlying mechanisms in search of agents that will
  702. block the atrophy. Repeated restraint stress in rats for 3 weeks causes changes in the hippocampal formation
  703. that include suppression of 5-HT1A receptor binding and atrophy of dendrites of CA3 pyramidal neurons, as
  704. well as impairment of initial learning of a radial arm maze task. <strong>
  705. Because serotonin is released by stressors and may play a role in the actions of stress on nerve cells,
  706. we investigated the actions of agents that facilitate or inhibit serotonin reuptake.</strong> Tianeptine
  707. is known to enhance serotonin uptake, and we compared it with fluoxetine, an inhibitor of 5-HT reuptake, as
  708. well as with desipramine. <strong>Tianeptine treatment (10 mg/kg/day) prevented the stress-induced atrophy
  709. of dendrites of CA3 pycamidal neurons,</strong> whereas neither fluoxetine (10 mg/kg/day) nor
  710. desipramine (10 mg/kg/day) had any effect. <strong>Tianeptine treatment also prevented the stress-induced
  711. impairment of radial maze learning.</strong>
  712. Because <strong>corticosterone- and stress-induced atrophy of CA3 dendrites is also blocked by phenytoin, an
  713. inhibitor of excitatory amino acid release and actions, these results suggest that serotonin released by
  714. stress or corticosterone may interact pre- or post-synaptically with glutamate released by stress or
  715. corticosterone, and that the final common path may involve interactive effects between serotonin and
  716. glutamate receptors on the dendrites of CA3 neurons innervated by mossy fibers from the dentate gyrus.
  717. We discuss the implications of these findings for treating cognitive impairments and the risk for
  718. dementia in the elderly.</strong>
  719. </p>
  720. © Ray Peat Ph.D. 2009. All Rights Reserved. www.RayPeat.com
  721. </body>
  722. </html>